Chinantecan is a Mesoamerican language family of the Oto-Manguean stock that comprises fourteen different languages spoken to the north of the state of Oaxaca, in Southern Mexico. Tlatepuzco Chinantec is the Chinantec language spoken by about 25,000 people in diaspora of the village of San Pedro Tlatepuzco, Oaxaca. The community was forced to move to different locations around their original settlement when a storm hit the village of San Pedro Tlatepuzco in 1928 and destroyed it. The language is also known as Palantla Chinantec, because an important part of the linguistic community relocated to San Juan Palantla.
Merrifield, W. R. and A. E. Anderson. 2007. Diccionario Chinanteco de la diáspora del pueblo antiguo de San Pedro Tlatepuzco, Oaxaca . [2nd Edition] (Serie de vocabularios y diccionarios indígenas “Mariano Silva y Aceves” 39). Mexico DF: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano. Available online.
Palancar, Enrique L. 2014. Revisiting the complexity of the Chinantecan verb conjugation classes, in Jean-Léo Léonard & Alain Kihm (eds.), Patterns in Meso-American Morphology, pp. 77-102. Paris: Michel Houdiard Editeur. Available online.
Baerman, Matthew and Enrique L. Palancar. Forthcoming. The organization of Chinantec tone paradigms. Cahiers de Grammaire. Available online.
Our notation of Tlatepuzco Chinantec differs slightly from the convention used in the dictionary by Merrifield and Anderson.
IPA notation | Source notation | Our notation |
---|---|---|
ɨ | ë | – |
ɘ | ø | – |
NASAL | Vn | – |
ʔ | h | ʔ |
h | j | h |
k | qu/_i,e | k |
c | ||
g | gu/_i,e | g |
g | ||
kw | cu/_i,e | kw |
qu | ||
tʃ | ch | ch |
ŋ | ŋ | ng |
Tone
Tone | Example | Gloss | |
---|---|---|---|
High | 3 | ta³ | 'work' |
Mid | 2 | kwøi² | 'maize' |
Low | 1 | hwø¹ | 'earth' |
Low-mid | 12 | lo¹² | 'pheasant' |
Low-high | 13 | lio¹³ | 'bulk' |
High-low | 31 | kai³¹ | 'street' |
The database gives the transitivity value of each verb, as given in the source material.
Verbs in Chinantec agree in animacy with the S or the O argument. Every entry in the database gives information about this animacy value:
There are two main types of verbs: stative verbs and dynamic verbs (also called ‘active verbs’). The database only includes information about dynamic verbs because stative verbs remain poorly understood to date.
Dynamic verbs fall into two types: (i) simple verbs, which consist of a mono-morphemic and (mostly) monosyllabic root:
húʔ¹² (tr) ‘bend’; ʔlianʔ¹² (tr an) ‘push’; gøi² (intr) ‘break down’, etc.
and (ii) bi-partite verbs (also called ‘binomial’), which originate from old V+V compounds, and are thus disyllabic and consist of two morphs. Bi-partite verbs have a number of inflectional idiosyncrasies of their own.
u²+ʔai¹² (intr an) ‘go out’; guu²+dsë́n¹³ (tr an) ‘ride’; mi²+lei¹³ (tr) ‘show’; etc.
The inflection of dynamic verbs can be very complex. Verbs have three main inflected stems, which the sources take as sufficient to reconstruct the full paradigm of a verb. These are:
Such inflected stems are built on a segmental base to which tone and pharyngealization (the so-called ballistic stress, represented by an acute accent) are added.
Inflected stems also convey information about grammatical person; the maximal person distinction involves four persons: 1SG, 1PL, 2 and 3. An example of the paradigm of a maximally inflected verb is presented below with the paradigm of the verb húʔ² (tr) ‘bend’. The shaded cell is the citation form used in Merrifield and Anderson's dictionary.
húʔ² (tr) ‘bend’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | húʔ¹² | húʔ¹² | húʔ¹² | húʔ² |
Completive stem | húʔ¹ | húʔ¹ | húʔ¹³ | húʔ² |
Irrealis stem | húʔ¹³ | húʔ¹³ | húʔ¹³ | húʔ² |
The different grammatical tenses are built by adding TAM prefixes (at times also in the form of prefix series and proclitics) to the different stems. For example:
Tense | Prefix | Stem | Example | |
---|---|---|---|---|
present | Ø- | Incompletive stem | húʔ¹²=hni | 'I'm bending' |
imperfect | mi³- | Incompletive stem | mi³-húʔ¹²=hni | 'I was bending' |
past | ka¹- | Completive stem | ka¹-húʔ¹=hni | 'I bent' |
hodiernal | na²- | Completive stem | na²-húʔ¹=hni | 'I bent (today)' |
future | Ø- | Irrealis stem | húʔ¹³=hni | 'I'll bend' |
potential | mi³- | Irrealis stem | mi³-húʔ¹³=hni | 'I'd bend' |
Tone (and ballistic stress) play a fundamental role in the making of the twelve cells which are said to be sufficient to reconstruct the paradigm of a verb. Tone information is given in the database for each verb.
WARNING: The database does not indicate ballistic stress because the way this prosodic feature is handled in the source data is not clear enough to allow us to reliably reconstruct paradigms. Ballistic stress has only been maintained in cases where the verb is uninflecting.
húʔ² (tr) ‘bend’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 12 | 12 | 12 | 2 |
Completive stem | 1 | 1 | 13 | 2 |
Irrealis stem | 13 | 13 | 13 | 2 |
Different verbs have different tone paradigms.
kán² (tr) ‘take’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 12 | 2 | 2 | 2 |
Completive stem | 1 | 2 | 3 | 2 |
Irrealis stem | 13 | 3 | 3 | 2 |
tsiʔ¹² (tr) ‘put’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 12 | 2 | 2 | 12 |
Completive stem | 1 | 2 | 3 | 1 |
Irrealis stem | 13 | 3 | 3 | 1 |
nëʔ¹² (tr) ‘swallow’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 12 | 12 | 12 | 12 |
Completive stem | 1 | 1 | 13 | 1 |
Irrealis stem | 13 | 13 | 13 | 1 |
Verbs such as húʔ² ‘bend’, kán² ‘take’, tsiʔ¹² ‘put’ or nëʔ¹² ‘swallow’ belong to different inflectional classes defined by tone patterns. There are many such classes. This information is indicated in the database. Merrifield and Anderson classify verbs like these in their own way. For example, the verbs above are said to belong to the following classes:
Verb | M&A's classification | |
---|---|---|
húʔ² | 'bend' | A-41a |
kán² | 'take' | A-42b |
tsiʔ¹² | 'put' | A-12d |
nëʔ¹² | 'swallow' | A-11a |
All the verbs above share the property of belonging to the paradigm type called ‘A’. Merrifield and Anderson propose four such paradigm types.
The ‘A’ type. Verbs of this type have maximal person distinctions.
húʔ² (tr) ‘bend’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 12 | 12 | 12 | 2 |
Completive stem | 1 | 1 | 13 | 2 |
Irrealis stem | 13 | 13 | 13 | 2 |
The ‘B’ type. Verbs of this type have two person distinctions: 3rd and non-3rd.
chionʔ¹² (tr) ‘stick’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 13 | 12 | ||
Completive stem | 1 | |||
Irrealis stem | 1 |
hógɁ¹² (intr) ‘come again’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 12 | 12 | ||
Completive stem | 1 | 1 | ||
Irrealis stem | 13 | 2 |
hónʔ² (tr) ‘sink’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 3 | 2 | ||
Completive stem | ||||
Irrealis stem |
tø²+gugɁ¹² (tr) ‘defecate’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 12 | 12 | ||
Completive stem | 1 | |||
Irrealis stem | 13 |
The ‘C' type. Verbs of this type have no person distinctions. C-type verbs include verbs with distinctions of aspect and mood and uninflecting verbs, which have a lexical tone throughout.
dsoʔ² (tr) ‘find’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 2 | |||
Completive stem | 2 | |||
Irrealis stem | 1 |
tan¹² (tr) ‘reach’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 12 | |||
Completive stem | 2 | |||
Irrealis stem | 1 |
rǿng² (tr) ‘mix’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 2 | |||
Completive stem | ||||
Irrealis stem |
The ‘D' type. Verbs of this type are like verbs of Type C, but they only inflect for the 3rd person.
Ɂlan¹² (intr) ‘bounce’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | – | 12 | ||
Completive stem | 1 | |||
Irrealis stem | 1 |
hí² (intr) ‘buzz’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | – | 2 | ||
Completive stem | ||||
Irrealis stem |
We analyse the conjugations patterns as being organised in a somewhat different way.
Pattern 1 – uninflecting
rǿng² (tr) ‘mix’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 2 | |||
Completive stem | ||||
Irrealis stem |
hí² (intr) ‘buzz’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | – | 2 | ||
Completive stem | ||||
Irrealis stem |
Pattern 2 – TAM distinctions only
dsoʔ² (tr) ‘find’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 2 | |||
Completive stem | 2 | |||
Irrealis stem | 1 |
Ɂlan¹² (intr) ‘bounce’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | – | 12 | ||
Completive stem | 1 | |||
Irrealis stem | 1 |
Pattern 3.a. – Two basic distinctions in person: 3rd vs. non-3rd person
hónʔ² (tr) ‘sink’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 3 | 2 | ||
Completive stem | ||||
Irrealis stem |
Pattern 3.b. – The 3rd person is invariable (i.e. Lexical), but non-3rd has distinctions in TAM
tø²+gugɁ¹² (tr) ‘defecate’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 12 | 12 | ||
Completive stem | 1 | |||
Irrealis stem | 13 |
Pattern 3.c. – The 3rd person has distinctions in TAM, but non-3rd is invariable
chionʔ¹² (tr) ‘stick’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 13 | 12 | ||
Completive stem | 1 | |||
Irrealis stem | 1 |
Pattern 3.d. – Both the 3rd person and the non-3rd have distinctions in TAM
hógɁ¹² (intr) ‘come again’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 12 | 12 | ||
Completive stem | 1 | 1 | ||
Irrealis stem | 13 | 2 |
Pattern 4 – Maximal number of TAM and person distinctions
húʔ² (tr) ‘bend’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | 12 | 12 | 12 | 2 |
Completive stem | 1 | 1 | 13 | 2 |
Irrealis stem | 13 | 13 | 13 | 2 |
To understand how inflectional classes are organized in Chinantec, we propose a different way of classifying verbs to that presented in the source material.
Under our proposal, the marking for 3rd person is treated as independent from the marking of other persons. Notice that húʔ² ‘bend’ and kán² ‘take’, on the one hand, and tsiʔ¹² ‘put’ and nëʔ¹² ‘swallow’, on the other, have the same marking for 3rd person, but kán² ‘take’ and tsiʔ¹² ‘put’ have the same marking for all other persons, while the same is true for húʔ² ‘bend’ and nëʔ¹² ‘swallow’. Hence for us, these verbs belong to two classes independently.
3rd person | Other persons | ||
---|---|---|---|
húʔ² | 'bend' | Uninflecting | Class IV |
kán² | 'take' | Uninflecting | Class II |
tsiʔ¹² | 'put' | Class 1 | Class II |
nëʔ¹² | 'swallow' | Class 1 | Class IV |
Under our analysis, there are 11 classes for the 3rd person, one of which is a class of verbs which show no tonal variation in the 3rd person.
Class | 3 INCPL | 3 CPL | 3 IRR |
---|---|---|---|
1 | 12 | 1 | 1 |
2 | 2 | 2 | 1 |
3 | 12 | 2 | 1 |
4 | 2 | 1 | 1 |
5 | 12 | 1 | 13 |
6 | 2 | 1 | 2 |
7 | 3 | 3 | 1 |
8 | 12 | 1 | 2 |
9 | 12 | 12 | 1 |
10 | 2 | 1 | 13 |
Uninflecting | Lexical tone |
Note that in this table, the first column labels the verbs as either Type A or Type B, corresponding to Merrifield and Anderson's classification. In addition to these classes, the database also signals lexemes which are unique or irregular.
Type | Class | 1SG INCPL | 1SG CPL | 1SG IRR | 2 INCPL | 2 CPL | 2 IRR | 1PL INCPL | 1PL CPL | 1PL IRR |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
A | i | 12 | 1 | 13 | 2 | 3 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 3 |
ii | 12 | 1 | 13 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 3 | |
iii | 12 | 1 | 13 | 12 | 1 | 13 | 2 | 3 | 3 | |
iv | 12 | 1 | 13 | 12 | 1 | 13 | 12 | 13 | 13 | |
v | 12 | 1 | 13 | 12 | 13 | 13 | 12 | 13 | 13 | |
vi | 12 | 1 | 13 | 12 | 12 | 13 | 12 | 13 | 13 | |
vii | 12 | 1 | 13 | 2 | 12 | 3 | 12 | 13 | 13 | |
viii | 12 | 1 | 13 | 12 | 3 | 13 | 12 | 13 | 13 | |
ix | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 1 | 3 | 12 | 13 | 13 | |
x | 2 | 2 | 3 | 12 | 12 | 13 | 12 | 13 | 13 | |
xi | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 12 | 13 | 13 | |
xii | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 13 | 13 | |
xiii | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 3 | |
xiv | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 1 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 3 | |
xv | 2 | 2 | 3 | 12 | 1 | 13 | 2 | 3 | 3 | |
xvi | 12 | 1 | 13 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 12 | 13 | 13 | |
xvii | 12 | 1 | 13 | 2 | 1 | 3 | 12 | 13 | 13 | |
xviii | 12 | 1 | 13 | 2 | 1 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 3 | |
xix | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | |
xx | 2 | 2 | 3 | 12 | 1 | 13 | 2 | 13 | 13 | |
xxi | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 3 | 2 | 13 | 13 | |
xxii | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 1 | 3 | 2 | 13 | 13 | |
xxiii | 2 | 2 | 3 | 12 | 12 | 13 | 2 | 13 | 13 | |
xxiv | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 12 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 3 | |
xxv | 2 | 2 | 3 | 12 | 3 | 13 | 12 | 13 | 13 | |
xxvi | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 12 | 1 | 1 | |
xxvii | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 13 | 13 | 13 | |
xxviii | 12 | 12 | 12 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 13 | 13 | 13 | |
B | a | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 |
b | 13 | 13 | 13 | 13 | 13 | 13 | 13 | 13 | 13 | |
c | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | |
d | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 3 | |
e | 12 | 13 | 13 | 12 | 13 | 13 | 12 | 13 | 13 | |
f | 12 | 1 | 13 | 12 | 1 | 13 | 12 | 1 | 13 |
In the inflection of a bi-partite verb, both morphs have inflectional properties. The first morph inflects according to a fixed pattern, while the second, the morphological root, may have inflectional forms similar to simple verbs.
hø²+tsóh¹² (tr) ‘stump with foot’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | hø²+ tsóh¹² | hø²+ tsóh² | hø²+ tsóh² | hø²+ tsóh¹² |
Completive stem | hø³+ tsóh¹ | hø³+ tsoh³ | hø³+ tsóh³ | hø¹+ tsóh¹ |
Irrealis stem | hø³+ tsóh¹³ | hø³+ tsóh³ | hø³+ tsóh³ | hø¹+ tsóh¹ |
Besides tone alternations, a number of verbs also have stems which undergo segmental changes in their inflection by way of stem alternation patterns. We refer to the different stems used in each alternation patterns using arbitrary labels:s A, B, C, etc.
dsanʔ¹² (tr) ‘find’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | A | A | A | A |
Completive stem | A | A | A | A |
Irrealis stem | B | B | B | B |
1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|
dsanʔ | dsanʔ | dsanʔ | dsanʔ |
dsanʔ | dsanʔ | dsanʔ | dsanʔ |
dsianʔ | dsianʔ | dsianʔ | dsianʔ |
hiúʔ² (tr) ‘hang’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | A | A | A | C |
Completive stem | A | A | A | C |
Irrealis stem | A | A | A | C |
1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|
hioʔ | hioʔ | hioʔ | hiuʔ |
hioʔ | hioʔ | hioʔ | hiuʔ |
hioʔ | hioʔ | hioʔ | hiuʔ |
tǿʔ² (tr) ‘call’ | 1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incompletive stem | A | A | B | C |
Completive stem | A | B | B | C |
Irrealis stem | A | A | B | C |
1SG | 2nd | 1PL | 3rd |
---|---|---|---|
tøa | tøa | te | tøʔ |
tøa | te | te | tøʔ |
tøa | tøa | te | tøʔ |
The phenomenon needs further study, but all forms involved and their distribution are given in the database. We have detected 17 such patterns.
Stem pattern | 1SG INCPL | 1SG CPL | 1SG IRR | 2 INCPL | 2 CPL | 2 IRR | 1PL INCPL | 1PL CPL | 1PL IRR | 3 INCPL | 3 CPL | 3 IRR |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. | A | A | A | A | B | A | B | B | B | A | A | A |
2. | A | A | A | A | B | A | B | B | B | B | B | B |
3. | A | A | A | C | A | C | C | C | C | A | A | A |
4. | A | A | B | A | A | B | A | A | B | A | A | B |
5. | A | A | A | A | A | A | A | A | A | C | C | C |
6. | A | A | A | A | B | A | B | B | B | C | C | C |
7. | A | A | A | C | B | C | B | B | B | A | A | A |
8. | D | D | D | C | A | C | C | C | C | D | D | D |
9. | A | A | A | C | B | C | B | B | B | D | D | D |
10. | A | A | A | C | B | C | B | B | B | B | B | B |
11. | C | C | D | E | E | F | A | A | B | A | A | B |
12. | C | C | B | D | D | B | A | A | B | A | A | B |
13. | A | A | A | C | D | C | B | B | B | A | A | A |
14. | C | C | C | D | E | D | B | B | B | A | A | A |
15. | B | B | B | C | C | C | A | A | A | A | A | A |
16. | A | A | A | C | C | C | B | B | B | A | A | A |
17. | C | C | C | A | A | A | B | B | B | A | A | A |